Theory of Mind

Cloggy

New Member
Joined
Feb 21, 2005
Messages
4,703
Reaction score
0
From here...

Theory of Mind: Language and Cognition in Deaf Children
by Brenda Schick, Jill de Villiers, Peter de Villiers, and Bob Hoffmeister

What does it mean to say that a child has a Theory of Mind?

As young children mature, they develop an understanding of themselves and other people as psychological beings who think, know, want, feel, and believe. They come to understand that what they think or believe may be different from what another person thinks and believes. They also learn that much of our behavior is motivated or caused by our knowledge and beliefs.

Simon’s Cake

An understanding of Theory of Mind might be best illustrated by an example, using a story.

Simon and his father made a cake together. But Simon wanted to go play and eat the cake later. So he put the cake in the cupboard. Then Simon left to play. The father thought that the icing on the cake might melt in the cupboard so he moved it to the refrigerator. Then the father left to go shopping. Later, Simon came home and he wanted to eat his cake. Where will Simon look for the cake?

You probably responded that Simon would look for the cake in the cupboard. He had put it there, and he didn’t know that his father had moved it. Your answer shows that you have a Theory of Mind. You can distinguish between what you know and the false belief, or misunderstanding, that Simon has.

Children around age 3, however, will answer this question incorrectly. They typically respond that the boy will look for the cake in the refrigerator, because that is where it is. At this age, children have difficulty distinguishing what they know from what others know. Children at this age are aware that people have thoughts, but they don’t realize that sometimes people’s thoughts and beliefs can be different from their own.

Major changes happen in the child’s understanding of other people’s minds around age 4. At this age, children can distinguish what they know from what others know, and they can correctly predict that a person’s behavior is dependent on what he thinks or knows, even when that belief is false. At about 4 years of age, a child can correctly predict that Simon would look in the cupboard, because that is where he left the cake.

Developmental psychologists call this a Theory of Mind because it shows that these children have an integrated set of concepts underlying their understanding of the mind. This is much the same as children’s "theories" in areas such as physics and biology. Children’s cognitive systems are not merely a collection of facts they have learned about the world. Rather, children develop an understanding that allows them to predict another person’s behavior based on what they know about that person’s thoughts and beliefs—just like you predicted that the boy would look for his cake in the cupboard. Like a scientific theory, a Theory of Mind allows us to interpret and predict other people’s actions by considering their thoughts, beliefs, desires, and emotions.

Theory of Mind and Deaf Children

Research has shown that children who are deaf have significant delay s in their understanding of a Theory of Mind. Previous investigators have speculated that the lack of access to conversations in the environment causes deaf children to miss important information about the world. But deaf children have some access to conversations, and, for the most part, they are socially competent, even when their language delays are severe. They do acquire a great deal of information about the world through visual means.

In addition, much of the research conducted on Theory of Mind skills in deaf children have relied solely on verbal tasks to assess their understanding. It is quite possible that deaf children have a Theory of Mind but don’t have sufficient language to understand the stories that are typically used to test them.

For very different reasons, children who have autism also have delays in developing a Theory of Mind. For the deaf child, the delay in acquiring a Theory of Mind is caused by lack of access to language, not by any fundamental cognitive problems. With the child with autism, the delays are a fundamental part of the disability, caused by cognitive differences in the child.

How Do Language Skills Relate to a Theory of Mind?

A recent investigation of Theory of Mind skills in children who are deaf (Language and Theory of Mind, funded by NIDCD to de Villiers, de Villiers, Schick, and Hoffmeister) attempted to determine whether deaf children were equally delayed in tasks that used language and those that didn’t. It also explored what aspects of the children’s development might be related to their understanding of the mind. The study included 176 children with a profound hearing loss representing three groups: 86 children being educated orally (53 with hearing aids, 33 with cochlear implants), 41 deaf children with hearing families learning American Sign Language (ASL), and 48 deaf children with deaf families exposed to ASL from birth. Unlike most previous studies, all children using ASL were tested by deaf adults who were native signers of ASL, and interpreters were not used.

Not surprisingly, the deaf children with deaf parents performed much like hearing children, while the children with hearing parents were significantly delayed in their understanding of a Theory of Mind. The results also show that the deaf children who were delayed in Theory of Mind were equally delayed in both the verbal tasks and the tasks that required minimal language. This means that the children lack some fundamental understanding of how the mind works regardless of whether the task requires language to understand it.

The study also revealed that the language skills in the deaf children were directly related to their Theory of Mind skills. However, it wasn’t the children’s general language skills but rather vocabulary skills and the specific ability to comprehend syntactic complements that predicted Theory of Mind skills. That is, if a child can understand sentences such as, "He thought his cake was in the cupboard," he is more likely to understand and predict behavior premised on a false belief, like that of the boy in the story. Children who had more advanced language skills were far more likely to pass the Theory of Mind tasks.

What this means is that the language delays that are typically observed in children who are deaf are causally related to delays in major aspects of cognitive development. Children who are not able to understand complex syntactic forms like complements have difficulty understanding how their own thoughts and beliefs may differ from those around them.

How Might Delays in Theory of Mind Development Affect Other Areas of Development?

There are several domains of development where Theory of Mind skills may be a prerequisite or foundation for later development. First, it is very likely that Theory of Mind skills play a central role in children’ s understanding and production of narratives. Bruner distinguishes between the "landscape of action" and the "landscape of consciousness," both essential to narration. The fact that many deaf children appear to be delayed in their development of Theory of Mind skills may contribute to impoverished understanding of stories and so to their widely reported delays in reading skills. While many deaf children have difficulties with English grammar and vocabulary, these problems may not be the only cause of reading delays.

Research shows that even preschool storybooks have a great deal of content related to desires and beliefs of the characters. For example, Little Red Riding Hood has a false belief in that she thinks the wolf is her grandmother, but as readers, we know that the wolf is deceiving her and intends to eat her too. In order to appreciate the story, and its landscape of consciousness, a child must be able to see beyond the action and understand that Little Red Riding Hood doesn’t know what we know. Otherwise the story becomes a tale of action where Little Red Riding Hood takes food to her grandmother, but for some unexplainable reason, a wolf is there instead. Much of the sense of the story is lost.

Second, it is possible that Theory of Mind skills are important for the development of social interaction skills, particularly those skills required in schools. Astington and Pelletier argue that there may be a relation between children’s level of Theory of Mind development and their ability to learn by instruction and collaboration. They suggest that Theory of Mind understanding is also linked to the development of scientific thinking and critical thinking.

In short, education requires children to talk about mutual understandings and misunderstandings, to reflect on their own beliefs as well as others, and to shift perspectives when evidence suggests that another point of view is valid. All of these require Theory of Mind skills.

How Can We Help Children to Learn Theory of Mind Skills?

There are many ways that adults can likely facilitate a child’s acquisition of a Theory of Mind. Pretend play and role-playing allow children to escape from the reality of objects and roles. In order to do this, children may need to create separate cognitive representations for what is real and what is imaginary. Talking about past events also has been found to be related to Theory of Mind skills in children, probably because children need to discuss decontextualized events. As mentioned, even preschool books contain numerous references and opportunities to explore how the mind works. And life itself presents many situations where people forget where they put something, misunderstand and tease each other, imagine what could be, and discuss opinions and perspectives. Discussing these events with children will help them acquire both the language of the mind as well as concepts that underlie a mature Theory of Mind.

Finally, research on the development of Theory of Mind skills with children who have limited access to language shows us the powerful role language has in developing fundamental social and cognitive skills. While many professionals are quite aware of how important language is in our lives for communication and learning world knowledge, research on Theory of Mind shows that language provides the scaffolding to understand how minds work.

-----------------------------------------

Brenda Schick is an associate professor of speech, language, and hearing sciences at the University of Colorado at Boulder. She studies the development of ASL and the relationship between language and cognitive development in deaf children. Contact her by e-mail at Brenda.Schick@colorado.edu .

Jill de Villiers is Sophia and Austin Smith Professor of Psychology and Philosophy at Smith College. She is currently studying the impact of language acquisition on cognitive development as well as the development of new diagnostic tests for language assessment with particular reference to African American English. Contact her by e-mail at jdevil@science.smith.edu .

Peter de Villiers is Sophia and Austin Smith Professor of Psychology at Smith College. He is currently studying the interactions between language acquisition, cognitive development, and literacy in deaf children and pragmatics assessment. Contact him by e-mail at PDEVILLI@science.smith.edu .

Robert Hoffmeister is the director of the Programs in Deaf Studies and the Center for the Study of Communication and the Deaf at Boston University. He is currently studying the relationship between learning, language development (ASL and English), and literacy in deaf children and the assessment of language in deaf children. Contact him by e-mail at rhoff@ACS.BU.EDU .

-----------------------------------------

References

Astington, J. W. (1993). The child’s discovery of the mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Astington, J., & Pelletier, J. (1996). The language of the mind: Its role in teaching and learning. In D. R. Olson & N. Torrance (Eds.), The handbook of education and human development: New models of learning, teaching, and schooling (pp. 593–619). Cambridge, MA: Blackwell Publishers Ltd.

Bruner, J. (1990). Acts of meaning. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Bruner, J. (1999). The intentionality of referring. In P. D. Zelazo, J. W. Astington, & D. R. Olson (Eds.), Developing theories of intention (pp. 329–340). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

de Villiers, J. G., & de Villiers, P. (2000). Linguistic determinism and the understanding of false beliefs. In P. Mitchell & K. Riggs (Eds.), Children’s reasoning and the mind (pp. 189–226). Hove, UK: Psychology Press.

de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., Schick, B., & Hoffmeister, R. (2000). Theory of mind development in signing and non-signing Deaf children: The impact of sign language on social-cognition. Paper presented at the Seventh International Conference on Theoretical Issues in Sign Language Research, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.

Gardner, H. (1991). The unschooled mind. New York: Basic Books.

Nelson, K. (1996). Language in cognitive development: The emergence of the mediated mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Peterson, C., & Siegal, M. (1999). Representing inner worlds: Theory of mind in autistic, deaf, and normal hearing children. Psychological Science, 10(2), 126–129.

Russell, P. A., Hosie, J. A., Gray, C. D., Scott, C., Hunter, N., Banks, J. S., & Macaulay, M. C. (1998). The development of theory of mind in deaf children. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry & Allied Disciplines, 39(6), 903–910.

Schick, B., de Villiers, P., de Villiers, J., & Hoffmeister, R. (2001). Language, literacy, and Theory of Mind in deaf children: What we know and what it means. Presented at the annual meeting of the American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, New Orleans, LA.

Schick, B., Hoffmeister, R., de Villiers, P., & de Villiers, J. (2000). American Sign Language and Theory of Mind in Deaf children with Deaf or hearing parents. Paper presented at the Seventh International Conference on Theoretical Issues in Sign Language Research, Amsterdam, The Netherlands.


Just as information....
 
Another interesting article.....
Language and theory-of-mind development in prelingually deafened children with cochlear implants: a preliminary investigation
Catrin E Macaulay, Ruth M Ford *
University of Wales Swansea, Singleton Park, Swansea SA2 8PP, Wales, UK

.................

Abstract
One cognitive domain in which deaf children are known to lag behind their hearing peers is that of theory of mind. Impoverished early conversational experience and deprivation of discussion about intangible mental states are seen as likely contributors to this deficit. The present study explored the relation between language and theory-of-mind capabilities in prelingually deafened children aged 4 to 11 years who were cochlear implant users. The children completed theory-of-mind tasks that indexed their ability to reason about conflicting mental representations, namely the understanding of false belief and the distinction between appearance and reality. Controlling for age, language ability was found to correlate positively and reliably with theory-of-mind performance. Copyright © 2006 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.
 
From this article..
Theory-of-mind development in oral deaf children with cochlear implants or conventional hearing aids
Candida C. Peterson11University of Queensland1University of Queensland
C.C. Peterson, School of Psychology, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland, Australia 4072; Email: candi@psy.uq.edu.au
Abstract
Background: In the context of the established finding that theory-of-mind (ToM) growth is seriously delayed in late-signing deaf children, and some evidence of equivalent delays in those learning speech with conventional hearing aids, this study's novel contribution was to explore ToM development in deaf children with cochlear implants. Implants can substantially boost auditory acuity and rates of language growth. Despite the implant, there are often problems socialising with hearing peers and some language difficulties, lending special theoretical interest to the present comparative design.

Methods: A total of 52 children aged 4 to 12 years took a battery of false belief tests of ToM. There were 26 oral deaf children, half with implants and half with hearing aids, evenly divided between oral-only versus sign-plus-oral schools. Comparison groups of age-matched high-functioning children with autism and younger hearing children were also included.

Results: No significant ToM differences emerged between deaf children with implants and those with hearing aids, nor between those in oral-only versus sign-plus-oral schools. Nor did the deaf children perform any better on the ToM tasks than their age peers with autism. Hearing preschoolers scored significantly higher than all other groups. For the deaf and the autistic children, as well as the preschoolers, rate of language development and verbal maturity significantly predicted variability in ToM, over and above chronological age.

Conclusions: The finding that deaf children with cochlear implants are as delayed in ToM development as children with autism and their deaf peers with hearing aids or late sign language highlights the likely significance of peer interaction and early fluent communication with peers and family, whether in sign or in speech, in order to optimally facilitate the growth of social cognition and language.
More food for thought
 
I find this very interesting. I saw a BBC documentary on child development last year and they covered theory of mind in one episode. I realised the other day that my daughter who is four, has got to this stage in development.
I was walking her home from pre-school, when she saw a couple of school girls picking dandelion clocks and blowing at them. She turned to me and said "who are they?"
I said "I don't know. They are strangers. Strangers are people that we haven't met before."
She paused thoughtfully and then said "We are strangers to them too, aren't we?"

It really shows how early language development is very important to developing ToM from the perspective of a deaf child. I'm sure that everyone here will agree with that. Thanks for posting!
 
From this article..

More food for thought

Once again, I'd really appreciate a link, because I need to know what control group was used in comparison for the results obtained. I know that you don't think that is pertinent information, but it really is necessary to interpret the results. Results are all you have provided.
 
I find this very interesting. I saw a BBC documentary on child development last year and they covered theory of mind in one episode. I realised the other day that my daughter who is four, has got to this stage in development.
I was walking her home from pre-school, when she saw a couple of school girls picking dandelion clocks and blowing at them. She turned to me and said "who are they?"
I said "I don't know. They are strangers. Strangers are people that we haven't met before."
She paused thoughtfully and then said "We are strangers to them too, aren't we?"

It really shows how early language development is very important to developing ToM from the perspective of a deaf child. I'm sure that everyone here will agree with that. Thanks for posting!

Achieving theory of mind is the ability to place oneself in the position of anopther, and then infer what their perspective would be. It is amazing to watch children develop this milestone. And while it is dependent upon language and interaction to a large degree, deaf children, even if delayed in passing this developmental milestone, do achieve theory of mind. In fact, most deaf adults that I know are remarkably adept at aadapting themselves to situations in which they must take the perspective of another. For instance, in assessing who will understand sign communications, and who will
understand only oral communications. It was theory of mind at work when my son assumed I was deaf because I signed, and that others who spoke were not deaf. While a limited assessment based on a restricted definition of deafness, it was theory of mind that allowed for that recognition.
 
...........
I was walking her home from pre-school, when she saw a couple of school girls picking dandelion clocks and blowing at them.
She turned to me and said "who are they?"
I said "I don't know. They are strangers. Strangers are people that we haven't met before."
She paused thoughtfully and then said
"We are strangers to them too, aren't we?"
......!
Beautiful example!!
 
Last edited:
My co worker told me about this 2 years ago..I thought it was very interesting and makes sense.

I observed my students who were language delayed last year on this..even at 8 years old, many of them shown that they hadnt developed Theory of Mind yet. :(
 
.............In fact, most deaf adults that I know are remarkably adept at aadapting themselves to situations in which they must take the perspective of another. ......

Nobody is saying anything to the contrairy..
 
Followed the links. I get it now. You are simply doing a Google search and pulling up any abstract you think supports your position. And here I thought you were actually reading full research article with all of the information necessary to interpret. Abstract are only used as a guide to find the article, cloggy, and are not useful in and of themselves for any other purpose. They cannot be used as a supportive argument.

Try going to a library, and pulling up entire articles, reading them, and then trying to understand the results. This is exactly what I am talking about when I refer to forming opinions without all of the qualifying and necessary information.
 
Followed the links. I get it now. You are simply doing a Google search and pulling up any abstract you think supports your position.
What position is that...... it wasn't aware that I was trying to support my position....
And here I thought you were actually reading full research article with all of the information necessary to interpret.
Actually, I did, I bought one of the articles. However, I cannot post the complete article when they have to be bought. I can post it when it's public. (like the first one.)
Abstract are only used as a guide to find the article, cloggy, and are not useful in and of themselves for any other purpose. They cannot be used as a supportive argument.
Try going to a library, and pulling up entire articles, reading them, and then trying to understand the results. This is exactly what I am talking about when I refer to forming opinions without all of the qualifying and necessary information.
.... but who was talking about "supportive argument".... I just started a thread about "Theory of Mind", and provided some articles.

Why are you so gung-ho on "supportive argumentation"....

Actually, the difference between R2D2's post and yours says it all:

R2D2:
I find this very interesting. I saw a BBC documentary on child development last year and they covered theory of mind in one episode. I realised the other day that my daughter who is four, has got to this stage in development.
I was walking her home from pre-school, when she saw a couple of school girls picking dandelion clocks and blowing at them. She turned to me and said "who are they?"
I said "I don't know. They are strangers. Strangers are people that we haven't met before."
She paused thoughtfully and then said "We are strangers to them too, aren't we?"

It really shows how early language development is very important to developing ToM from the perspective of a deaf child. I'm sure that everyone here will agree with that. Thanks for posting!


You:
I'd really appreciate a link, because I need to know what control group was used in comparison for the results obtained. I know that you don't think that is pertinent information, but it really is necessary to interpret the results. Results are all you have provided.
And to think that I gave you all the information to find the article...
btw... is your google working again...
 
Back
Top